月旦知識庫
 
  1. 熱門:
 
首頁 臺灣期刊   法律   公行政治   醫事相關   財經   社會學   教育   其他 大陸期刊   核心   重要期刊 DOI文章
中華心理學刊 本站僅提供期刊文獻檢索。
  【月旦知識庫】是否收錄該篇全文,敬請【登入】查詢為準。
最新【購點活動】


篇名
華人青少年前期兒童之間接攻擊行為的反應評鑑與情緒經驗
並列篇名
The Response Evaluation and Emotional Experiences of Indirect Aggression: Findings from Children during Early Adolescence in a Culturally Chinese Context
作者 王明傳雷庚玲
中文摘要
本研究旨在發展一套有別於問卷形式的間接攻擊結構性訪談測量程序。依據社會訊息處理模式,本研究預期此訪談工具可區別青少年前期不同同儕地位的兒童對於間接攻擊行為的反應評鑑。依據效益風險率假設,本研究預期男女生在遭遇間接攻擊後的情緒反應具顯著差異,也預期間接攻擊行為的反應評鑑具性別與同儕地位之交互作用效果。研究一共有56位國小高年級兒童參加個別訪談,結果顯示間接攻擊行為反應評鑑的同儕地位效果量為中等,且遭遇間接攻擊行為受害的情緒經驗具有性別差異。研究二共有60位不同社會計量地位之國二生參加個別訪談,結果顯示性別與間接攻擊行為的反應評鑑之間的關係會受社會計量地位所調節,且遭遇間接攻擊的情緒經驗具有性別差異。研究三共有72位高、低間接攻擊傾向之小六兒童參加個別訪談,結果顯示間接攻擊行為反應評鑑的性別和間接攻擊傾向之交互作用效果不顯著,且遭遇間接攻擊行為的情緒經驗具有性別差異。這三個研究都發現不同性別的兒童雖然在遭遇間接攻擊時的憤怒情緒沒有差異,但對於間接攻擊之困擾和難過情緒卻顯著不同。本研究另顯示性別與間接攻擊行為的反應評鑑之間的關係會受社會計量地位指標影響,卻與間接攻擊傾向程度的同儕地位指標沒有顯著關係。
英文摘要
Although the understanding of indirect aggression has been significantly advanced in recent years, past research still suffers from a few limitations. Firstly, data should be collected through methods other than questionnaires that dominate the recent findings. Secondly, research has yet to further clarify the processes that lead to the emotions and the response evaluation while being victimized by indirect aggression in peer context. Thirdly, most prior studies focused on children in middle childhood, leaving the effect of indirect aggression on children during early adolescence less explored. Finally, the vast majority of research has been conducted in Western cultural contexts, which may conceive the meaning of indirect aggression differently from culturally Chinese societies. Therefore, the specific goal of this research was to examine the response evaluation and the emotional reactions to victimization of indirect aggression among culturally Chinese children at the stage of early adolescence by means of a newly developed structured interview with card-sorting procedures. The interaction effect between gender and peer status on the response evaluation was also of interest according to the effect-to- danger ratio hypothesis. Fifty-six children nominated by their head teachers as popular or rejected (mean age = 11.60 years) participated in Study 1. During the interview, a vignette depicting indirect aggression was visually and auditorily presented, and then a series of questions concerning personal experiences, including emotional reactions, were presented. Subjects evaluation on the effectiveness of various kinds of indirect aggression strategies was then probed through a deck of 15 cards each stating a strategy of indirect aggression. Results showed that the gender by peer status interaction on response evaluation was not significant. However, rejected children enumerated marginally more indirect aggression strategies as effective than their popular counterparts. Study 1 also confirmed the effect-to-danger ratio hypothesis and showed that girls felt more troubled and sad than did boys in response to indirect aggression, while degree of anger revealed no gender differences. Sixty children nominated by same-gender same-class peers as popular or rejected (mean age = 13.98 years) participated in Study 2, where the interview procedure was similar to Study 1 except for minor modifications. The number of indirect aggression strategies that popular and rejected subjects enumerated as effective was not different. Next, to take the statistical power issue into account, data from Study 1 and Study 2 were pooled. Results indicated that rejected subjects enumerated more indirect aggression strategies as effectively than their popular counterparts. Further, Study 2 found a significant interaction effect of peer status and gender on response evaluation. Compared with popular boys, rejected boys were more likely to adopt indirect aggression strategies to cope with indirect aggression. By contrast, rejected girls did not favor indirect aggression strategies more than popular girls. As for emotional reactions to victimization by indirect aggression, Study 2 replicated the findings in Study 1 that confirmed the effect-to-danger ratio hypothesis. Study 3 attempted to further validate the structured interview by comparing the responses between children designated through peer estimation technique as either highly or less indirect- aggressive and 72 children (mean age = 12.8 years) were recruited. Results indicated those who were highly indirect-aggressive did not enumerate more indirect aggression strategies as effectively than did the less aggressive counterparts. Further, the interaction effect of peer- estimated indirect aggression tendency and gender was not significant. It is suspected that the highly indirect- aggressive children might be better or at least not worse at social intelligence, which had buffered them from displaying biases or deficiencies in social information processing. Study 3, again, replicated the findings in Study 1 and Study 2 that girls were more troubled and sad than boys but felt similar degree of anger compared to boys while being victimized by indirect aggression. Findings from the above studies were discussed based on both the social information-processing model and the effect-to-danger ratio hypothesis. The incompatible results among the three studies were discussed according to a developmental perspective.
起訖頁 71-90
關鍵詞 間接攻擊關係攻擊同儕地位同儕關係社會訊息處理indirect aggressionrelational aggressionpeer statuspeer relationshipsocial information- processing
刊名 中華心理學刊  
期數 200803 (50:1期)
出版單位 台灣心理學會
該期刊-上一篇 兒童對同儕言語挑釁之受傷與易怒敏感度:社會計量差異與發展差異
該期刊-下一篇 以魏氏成人智力量表第三版四因素為基礎的簡短式測驗組型:臨床應用指引
 

新書閱讀



最新影音


優惠活動




讀者服務專線:+886-2-23756688 傳真:+886-2-23318496
地址:臺北市館前路28 號 7 樓 客服信箱
Copyright © 元照出版 All rights reserved. 版權所有,禁止轉貼節錄